You could just go at it the other way. Guess-and-check is one of the basic math strategies taught in schools, and is easy to apply to these questions:
‘I need something to answer this… 0.10 sounds good, but let’s check it first. 1+0.1 means the bat costs 1.10, and 1.10+0.10 = 1.2 - what a minute, that’s not 1.10! I better try some other values—I guess this isn’t so obvious after all!’
It’d be interesting if getting the wrong answer first is the quickest method of getting the right answer.
I recently read something like this, though I can’t remember where. The experiment went roughly like so:
Subjects were divided into two groups. In one, each subject was given 15 seconds to memorize an answer to a question for several seconds, and their performance recalling the answer later was recorded. In the other, each was asked to guess the answer, and was then given 7 or 8 or so seconds to memorize the correct answer. The time difference was to account for the time during which the second group’s members thought about the question.
So each person was exposed to the question for 15 seconds, in the first group, they were exposed to the answer for those same 15 seconds, in the second group, for half that.
The second group was better at recalling the answers.
I found a set of five experiments similar to the one I described. Getting the wrong answer first appears to be a good method to get to the right answer.
Specifically, we evaluated the benefits of testing novel science instructional content before learning. Thus, the likelihood of failed tests was high, but we were able to extend our theory of testing to better understand whether trying and failing on test questions actually improved learners’ longer term retention of subsequently presented information...in the current study, the prequestions required participants to produce nouns or descriptive statements that they were unlikely to be able to answer on the basis of prior knowledge (e.g., “What is total colorblindness caused by brain damage called?”). This allowed us to isolate and examine the effects of unsuccessful retrieval attempts.
...
Previous research has demonstrated the memory benefits of successfully answering test questions. The five experiments reported herein provide evidence for the power of tests as learning events even when the tests are unsuccessful. Participants benefited from being tested before studying a passage—a pretesting effect—although they did not answer the test questions correctly on the initial test, as compared with being allowed additional study time. Furthermore, the benefits of pretests persisted after a 1-week delay.
...
The current research suggests that tests can be valuable learning events, even if learners cannot answer test
questions correctly, as long as the tested material has educational value and is followed by instruction that provides answers to the tested questions.
You could just go at it the other way. Guess-and-check is one of the basic math strategies taught in schools, and is easy to apply to these questions:
‘I need something to answer this… 0.10 sounds good, but let’s check it first. 1+0.1 means the bat costs 1.10, and 1.10+0.10 = 1.2 - what a minute, that’s not 1.10! I better try some other values—I guess this isn’t so obvious after all!’
Right. I know how to get to a right answer but didn’t understand Tordmor’s expression.
Starting with
bat + ball = 1.10
bat = 1 + ball
substitute one into the other to eliminate it
1 + ball + ball = 1.10
simplify
1 + 2 ball = 1.10
then solve for ball
ball = (1.10 − 1)/2
then compute
ball = 0.05
This is literally how I would solve this problem. So you can see why I’m surprised people can answer it correctly on the fly.
The way it went in my head:
Huh, that’s obvious, it’s 1. Oh wait, ‘more than.’ So it’s half the remaining .10.
(Although I would say it took less time than reading that sentence takes.)
It’d be interesting if getting the wrong answer first is the quickest method of getting the right answer.
I recently read something like this, though I can’t remember where. The experiment went roughly like so:
Subjects were divided into two groups. In one, each subject was given 15 seconds to memorize an answer to a question for several seconds, and their performance recalling the answer later was recorded. In the other, each was asked to guess the answer, and was then given 7 or 8 or so seconds to memorize the correct answer. The time difference was to account for the time during which the second group’s members thought about the question.
So each person was exposed to the question for 15 seconds, in the first group, they were exposed to the answer for those same 15 seconds, in the second group, for half that.
The second group was better at recalling the answers.
I found a set of five experiments similar to the one I described. Getting the wrong answer first appears to be a good method to get to the right answer.
I get it, thanks!