Playing devil’s advocate: Archaic spelling rules allow you to quickly gauge other people’s intelligence, which is useful. It causes society to respect stupid people less, by providing objective evidence of their stupidity.
But I don’t actually think the benefits outweigh the costs there, and the signal is confounded by things like being a native English-speaker.
Spelling is more a gauge of how attentive you were in early schooling than of how intelligent you are. It’s basically a form of conspicuous consumption of the scarce resources of childhood attention and teaching time.
The cultural notion that bad spelling is an indicator of stupidity is self-reinforcing, though: it prevents English from undergoing spelling reforms like those German, Spanish, Russian, and many other languages have had, because any “reformed” spelling will necessarily look like ignorant spelling.
Because English spelling is unusually difficult, it is a challenge. Because it is a challenge, people who have mastered it care about the fact that they have mastered it. And because of that, it can’t be made easier.
any “reformed” spelling will necessarily look like ignorant spelling
It is much easier to do a spelling reform in a mostly illiterate country, where you can defend it by saying “look, most people can’t read, we need to make it easier for them”. Having a monarchy or dictatorship also helps to introduce the changes quickly and everywhere.
I’d assume it’s a measure of both attentiveness and intelligence. And also of how much reading you did when young. I expect all these things correlate enough to make it hard to disentangle them, but just on first principles it seems obvious that you’ll learn spellings better (1) if you generally learn things better, (2) if you’re exposed to more correct spellings, and (3) if you’re paying more attention to spelling relative to other things.
I agree about the difficulties of spelling reform. Perhaps sufficient support from high-status intellectual literary people might get past the “reform looks like ignorance” problem. Strong support from George Bernard Shaw wasn’t enough for English in the early 20th century; perhaps it could be done with a large enough coalition of obviously expert people, or incrementally with each smaller step perhaps being easier to accept.
(Whether it would be a good idea, I don’t know. I’ve not seen evidence that the difficulty of spelling in English—which I think is one of the hardest-to-spell major languages—causes much actual harm. And yes, for the avoidance of doubt, the Mark Twain thing [EDITED to add: very probably not actually written by Mark Twain] I linked to was written as a joke and not a serious proposal; I linked to it because I think it’s funny.)
Playing devil’s advocate: Archaic spelling rules allow you to quickly gauge other people’s intelligence, which is useful. It causes society to respect stupid people less, by providing objective evidence of their stupidity.
But I don’t actually think the benefits outweigh the costs there, and the signal is confounded by things like being a native English-speaker.
Spelling is more a gauge of how attentive you were in early schooling than of how intelligent you are. It’s basically a form of conspicuous consumption of the scarce resources of childhood attention and teaching time.
The cultural notion that bad spelling is an indicator of stupidity is self-reinforcing, though: it prevents English from undergoing spelling reforms like those German, Spanish, Russian, and many other languages have had, because any “reformed” spelling will necessarily look like ignorant spelling.
Because English spelling is unusually difficult, it is a challenge. Because it is a challenge, people who have mastered it care about the fact that they have mastered it. And because of that, it can’t be made easier.
I think it’s a gauge of how much you read. Bad spelling is not an indicator of stupidity, but an indicator of not having read enough.
It is much easier to do a spelling reform in a mostly illiterate country, where you can defend it by saying “look, most people can’t read, we need to make it easier for them”. Having a monarchy or dictatorship also helps to introduce the changes quickly and everywhere.
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Reforms_of_Russian_orthography
Today I learned: Russian once had a letter for “th”, but it was removed and replaced by either “f” or “t”.
Indeed. Look at the rejected recent German orthography reform – and the changes were (relatively) minor.
Or the messed up Slovak orthography reform from the ’90s – and that was mostly a few acutes here and there.
I’d assume it’s a measure of both attentiveness and intelligence. And also of how much reading you did when young. I expect all these things correlate enough to make it hard to disentangle them, but just on first principles it seems obvious that you’ll learn spellings better (1) if you generally learn things better, (2) if you’re exposed to more correct spellings, and (3) if you’re paying more attention to spelling relative to other things.
I agree about the difficulties of spelling reform. Perhaps sufficient support from high-status intellectual literary people might get past the “reform looks like ignorance” problem. Strong support from George Bernard Shaw wasn’t enough for English in the early 20th century; perhaps it could be done with a large enough coalition of obviously expert people, or incrementally with each smaller step perhaps being easier to accept.
(Whether it would be a good idea, I don’t know. I’ve not seen evidence that the difficulty of spelling in English—which I think is one of the hardest-to-spell major languages—causes much actual harm. And yes, for the avoidance of doubt, the Mark Twain thing [EDITED to add: very probably not actually written by Mark Twain] I linked to was written as a joke and not a serious proposal; I linked to it because I think it’s funny.)